Page 7 - Principles of instructional design
P. 7

The Outcomes of Instruction 45
         Goodnow, and Austin, 1956). Rothkopf (1971) has named them "mathema-
         genic behaviors"; Skinner (1968) "self-management behaviors." One expects
         that such skills will improve over a relatively long period of time as the in-
         dividual engages in more and more studying, learning, and thinking. An ex-
         ample shown in Table 3-1  is the cognitive strategy of using images as links
        \o connect words in the learning of foreign-language vocabulary (Atkinson,
         1975).
           Provided it has previously been learned, a cognitive strategy may be selected
         by a learner as a mode of solving a novel problem. Often, for example, newly
         encountered problems can be efficiendy approached by working backward in
         stages beginning with the goal to be achieved by a solution. This "working
         backward" approach  is an example of a cognitive strategy. Intellectual skills
         (such as basic arithmetic operations) frequently have to be recalled by the learner
         and brought to bear upon a problem. But although these skills are essential, they
         are not sufficient. A mode of seeking a solution must also be used bv the learner,
         a cognitive strategy that he has practiced in the past, perhaps many times in a
         variety of situations.
          The most commonly occurring cognitive strategies are domain specific. For
         example, there are strategies for retaining information from reading, for aiding
         the solution of word problems in arithmetic, for helping the composition of
         effective sentences, and many others that focus on particular domains of learning
         tasks. However, some cognitive strategies are more general, like the process
         called inference or induction. Suppose that a student has become acquainted with
         magnetic attraction in a bar magnet—noting that a force is exerted by each pole
         of the magnet on certain kinds of metal objects. Then, the student is given some
         iron filings to sprinkle on a piece of paper placed over the magnet. When the
         paper  is tapped, the filings exhibit "lines of force" around each pole of the
         magnet. The student then verifies this observation in other situations, perhaps
         using other magnets and other kinds of metal objects. These observations,
         together with other knowledge, may lead to the induction of the idea of a
         magnetic field of force surrounding each pole of the magnet. It is important to
         note in this example that the student has not been told of the magnetic field
         beforehand or given instruction in "how to induce." But this kind of mental
         operation is carried out.
           Learning a cognitive strategy such as induction, however, is apparently not
         done on a single occasion. Instead, this kind of capability develops over fairly
         long periods of time. Presumablv, the learner must have a number of experi-
         ences with induction in widely different situations for the strategy to become
         dependably useful.
          When a learner becomes capable of induction, this strategy may be used in a
         great variety of other situations. Provided other requisite intellectual skills and
         information have been learned, an induction strategy may be used to arrive at an
         explanation of what makes smoke rise in the air, why pebbles in a stream are
         rounded and smooth, or what intention a writer had in composing an editorial
   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9   10   11   12