Page 61 - Zoo Animal Learning and Training
P. 61

References  33

  VetBooks.ir  alarm calls: speaking the same language or   Schneider, K.J. (1984). Dominance, predation,
                                                        and optimal foraging in White‐Throated
               learning another? Proceedings of the Royal
                                                        sparrow flocks. Ecology 65 (6): 1820.
               Society of London, Series B: Biological
               Sciences 276: 769–774.                 Seyfarth, R.M. and Cheney, D.L. (1986). Vocal
             Masataka, N. (1993). Effects of experience with   development in vervet monkeys. Animal
               live insects on the development of fear of   Behaviour 34: 1640–1658.
               snakes in squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus.   Shettleworth, S.J. (2001). Animal cognition
               Animal Behaviour 46: 741–746.            and animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour 61:
             Mateo, J.M. (1996). The development of     277–286.
               alarm‐call response behaviour in free‐living   Sih, A. and Christensen, B. (2001). Optimal
               juvenile Belding’s ground squirrel. Animal   diet theory: when does it work, and when
               Behaviour 52: 489–505.                   and why does it fail? Animal Behaviour 61
             Mateo, J.M. and Holmes, W.G. (1997).       (2): 379–390.
               Development of alarm‐call responses in   Sousa, C., Biro, D., and Matsuzawa, T. (2009).
               Belding’s ground squirrels: the role of dams.   Leaf‐tool use for drinking water by wild
               Animal Behaviour 54: 509–524.            chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): acquisition
             Menzel, R. and Giurfa, M. (2001). Cognitive   patterns and handedness. Animal Cognition
               architecture of a mini‐brain: the        12 (1): 115–125.
               honeybee. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5:   St Amant, R. and Horton, T.E. (2008).
               62–71.                                   Revisiting the definition of animal tool use.
             Menzel, R. and Müller, U. (1996). Learning and   Animal Behaviour 75 (4): 1199–1208.
               memory in honeybees: from behaviour to   Stryjek, R., Mioduszewska, B., Spaltabaka‐
               neural substrates. Annual Review of      Gędek, E., and Juszczak, G.R. (2018).
               Neuroscience 19: 379–404.                Wild Norway rats do not avoid predator
             Much, R.M., Breck, S.W., Lance, N.J., and   scents when collecting food in a familiar
               Callahan, P. (2018). An ounce of prevention:   habitat: a field study. Scientific Reports 8
               Quantifying the effects of non‐lethal tools   (1): 9475.
               on wolf behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour   Suzuki, S., Kuroda, S., and Nishihara, T. (1995).
               Science 203: 73–80.                      Tool‐set for termite‐fishing by chimpanzees
             Nowell, A.A. and Fletcher, A.W. (2008). The   in the Ndoki Forest, Congo. Behaviour 132
               development of feeding behaviour in wild   (3–4): 219–235.
               western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla   Svádová, K., Exnerová, A., Štys, P. et al. (2009).
               gorilla). Behaviour 145: 171–193.        Role of different colours of aposematic
             Ottoni, E.B. and Mannu, M. (2001). Semifree‐  insects in learning, memory and
               ranging Tufted Capuchins (Cebus apella)   generalization of naïve bird predators.
               spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts.   Animal Behaviour 77: 327–336.
               International Journal of Primatology 22 (3):   Takahasi, M. and Okanoya, K. (2010). Song
               347–358.                                 learning in wild and domesticated strains of
             Owings, D.H. and Leger, D.W. (1980). Chatter   white‐rumped munia, Lonchura striata,
               vocalizations of California ground squirrels:   compared by cross‐fostering procedures:
               predator‐and social‐role specificity.    domestication increases song variability by
               Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 54 (2):   decreasing strain‐specific bias. Ethology 116:
               163–184.                                 396–405.
             Ropert‐Coudert, Y., Kato, A., Wilson, R.P.,   Thornton, A. and McAuliffe, K. (2006).
               and Cannell, B. (2006). Foraging strategies   Teaching in wild meerkats. Science 313:
               and prey encounter rate of free‐ranging   227–229.
               Little Penguins. Marine Biology 149 (2):   Tierney, A.J. (1986). The evolution of learned
               139–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/        and innate behaviour: contributions from
               s00227‐005‐0188‐x.                       genetics and neurobiology to a theory of
   56   57   58   59   60   61   62   63   64   65   66