Page 107 - Zoo Animal Learning and Training
P. 107
Box A3 Makinng Sense oouScents: loactorry erception inuAnimals 79
VetBooks.ir impaired, despite the olfactory apparatus pre- changing olfactory conditions and pressures
where the importance and relevance of spe-
sumably remaining intact (Koss et al. 1988). In
animals, including humans, the olfactory sys-
responses to them, such plasticity is essential.
tem is a flexible processing network that is cific odours varies along with the appropriate
shaped and honed by learning. In a world of
References
Alberts, A.C. (1992). Pheromonal self‐ Keverne, E.B. (1999). The vomeronasal organ.
recognition in desert iguanas. Copeia 1992 Science 286 (5440): 716–720.
(1): 229–232. Koss, E., Weiffenbach, J.M., Haxby, J.V., and
Barja, I., de Miguel, F.J., and Bárcena, F. (2004). Friedland, R.P. (1988). Olfactory detection
The importance of crossroads in faecal and identification performance are
marking behaviour of the wolves (Canis dissociated in early Alzheimer’s disease.
lupus). Naturwissenschaften 91 (10): 489–492. Neurology 38 (8): 1228–1228.
Bradbury, J.W. and Vehrencamp, S.L. (1998). Lehrner, J.P., Walla, P., Laska, M., and Deecke,
Principles of Animal Communication. L. (1999). Different forms of human odor
Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer. memory: a developmental study.
Ebling, F.J. (1977). Hormonal control of Neuroscience Letters 272 (1): 17–20.
mammalian skin glands. In: Chemical Signals Rabin, M.D. (1988). Experience facilitates
in Vertebrates (eds. D. Müller‐Schwarze and olfactory quality discrimination. Perception
M.M. Mozell), 17–33. Boston, MA: Springer. & Psychophysics 44 (6): 532–540.
Eisenberg, J.F. and Kleiman, D.G. (1972). Rasmussen, L.E.L. and Schulte, B.A. (1998).
Olfactory communication in mammals. Chemical signals in the reproduction of
Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 3 Asian (Elephas maximus) and African
(1): 1–32. (Loxodonta africana) elephants. Animal
Epple, G., Belcher, A.M., Greenfield, K.L. et al. reproduction science 53 (1–4): 19–34.
(1987). Making sense out of scents‐species‐ Roberts, S.C. and Lowen, C. (1997). Optimal
differences in scent glands, scent marking patterns of scent marks in klipspringer
behavior and scent mark composition in the (Oreotragus oreotragus) territories. Journal
Callitrichidae. International Journal of of Zoology 243 (3): 565–578.
Primatology. 8 (5): 434–434. Uchida, N., Takahashi, Y.K., Tanifuji, M., and
Gillingham, J.C. and Clark, D.L. (1981). Snake Mori, K. (2000). Odor maps in the
tongue‐flicking: transfer mechanics to mammalian olfactory bulb: domain
Jacobson’s organ. Canadian Journal of organization and odorant structural
Zoology 59 (9): 1651–1657. features. Nature Neuroscience 3 (10): 1035.
Gosling, L.M., Roberts, S.C., Thornton, E.A., Vickers, N.J. (2000). Mechanisms of animal
and Andrew, M.J. (2000). Life history costs of navigation in odor plumes. The Biological
olfactory status signalling in mice. Behavioral Bulletin 198 (2): 203–212.
Ecology and Sociobiology 48 (4): 328–332. Wilson, D.A. and Stevenson, R.J. (2003). The
Hudson, R. and Distel, H. (1983). Nipple fundamental role of memory in olfactory
location by newborn rabbits: behavioural perception. Trends in Neurosciences 26 (5):
evidence for pheromonal guidance. 243–247.
Behaviour 85 (3): 260–274. Wright, G.A. and Schiestl, F.P. (2009). The
Jordan, N.R., Golabek, K.A., Apps, P.J. et al. evolution of floral scent: the influence of
(2013). Scent‐mark identification and olfactory learning by insect pollinators on
scent‐marking behaviour in African wild dogs the honest signalling of floral rewards.
(Lycaon pictus). Ethology 119 (8): 644–652. Functional Ecology 23 (5): 841–851.