Page 300 - The Welfare of Cattle
P. 300

herdsManshIP and huMan InteraCtIon                                          277


            to select to describe stockperson behavior. They were as follows: quick, dominating, aggressive,
            fearful, patient, careful, calm, determined, focused, insecure, careless, talks to the animals, cuddles
            the animals, inventive, nervous, boisterous, and including. The observers completed a visual assess-
            ment scale for each of the 17 variables to create numerical scores based on the location of the mark
            they drew on the scale. This is one method of converting subjective descriptions to more objective
            terms. In the same study, the researchers also had observers assess the behavior of the calves during
            stockperson interaction. Thirty-one descriptors were included in the list to describe the behavior
            of the calves during interaction with the stockperson. They were as follows: nervous, frustrated,
            fearful, enjoying, distressed, uncomfortable, friendly, content, sociable, uneasy, calm, confident,
            agitated, unwell, happy, scared, positively occupied, relaxed, boisterous, inquisitive, playful, tense,
            aggressive, bored, depressed, active,  lively, irritable, vigilant,  apathetic, and  indifferent. After
            observing the interaction of a stockperson and calf, the observer employed the same assessment
            method described above to create numerical values describing each of the 31 characteristics. This
            approach is inherently subjective as each individual observer will vary to some extent regarding
            specific level of any single descriptor they observe. However, the introduction of a means of con-
            verting a discrete outcome—was the descriptor observed versus not?—to a continuous outcome—
            what level of each descriptor was observed?—facilitates the calculation of statistics that can better
            describe the differences between observers.
               An additional challenge that requires attention as methods of testing human-animal interac-
            tion are developed and implemented is controlling for the ability of animals to recognize handlers
            and respond to handlers in different ways (de Passillé and Rushen, 2005). It is likely that common
            methods of quantifying animal responses to human interaction will emerge as the field of animal
            welfare science continues to mature. As these common methods arise, the following factors must
            be considered: (1) How well are the assessment methods applied in field versus laboratory settings?,
            (2) Do the assessment methods accurately capture and quantify animal behavior when applied over
            time?, (3) What are acceptable levels of repeatability and reliability for tests of human-animal inter-
            action?, and (4) What is an appropriate threshold between acceptable and unacceptable test results?.
               It is important to note that behavioral assessments may not be the only set of measures that
            are necessary to fully understand the quality of stockmanship (de Passillé and Rushen, 2005). The
            early debate over the relative importance behavioral versus physiological assessments of animal wel-
            fare states was discussed in detail by Duncan (1997). He described two primary schools of thought
            regarding animal welfare assessment, namely, Biological Function and Feelings. He described the
            Biological Function school of thought as an approach that was focused on assessing the operative and
            physiological states of the body as the primary means of understanding welfare states. The Feelings
            school of thought was focused on understanding the affective state of the animal. This approach did
            not consider the physiological state of the animal, but relied heavily on interpreting behavioral dis-
            plays instead. A reasonable argument can be made that a well-informed assessment of animal welfare
            should include a blend of both schools of thought. Such an approach aligns with a point made by de
            Passillé and Rushen (2005); that a full understanding of the implications of good or poor stockman-
            ship should weigh the behaviors an animal displays, but also consider other outcomes such as physi-
            ological responses, animal productivity, and final product quality attributes if possible.


                                   IMPOrtaNCe OF MaNaGeMeNt

            Prevention of abusive Behavior

               Abusive behavior is difficult to detect in animal welfare assessments for multiple reasons. First,
            people tend to display their best behavior when the feel that they are being monitored. In addition, a
            formal animal welfare assessment captures a limited view of the state of animal welfare at a farm.
   295   296   297   298   299   300   301   302   303   304   305