Page 188 - Deep Learning
P. 188

The Growth of Competence                 171

            away, off the coast of Marseilles, two divers, Albert Falco and Claude Wesley,
            entered Conshelf i, an undersea habitat designed and operated by the French
            research organization office Française de Recherches Sous-Marins under the
            direction of Captain J. Y. Costeau. Conshelf i was a two-person dwelling, 17
            feet long and 8 feet high, anchored a few feet above the sea floor at a depth of
            40 feet near the island of pomègues. Falco and Wesley lived and worked at a
            depth of 10 meters for seven days. in conjunction, these two pioneering efforts
            demonstrated that human beings could live and work underwater.
               A decade later, space explorers made the same point for the very different
                                   8
            environment of near space.  on May 14, 1973, the national Aeronautics and
            Space Administration (nASA), the U.S. space agency, launched Skylab, the
            first successful, manned space station. The three astronauts Charles Conrad Jr.,
            paul J. Weitz and Joseph p. Kerwin stayed in Skylab between May 25 and June
            22, thus becoming the first humans to live and work in space and to return
            safely to Earth.
               our hunter-gatherer ancestors cannot have bequeathed to us any genetic
            adaptations for flipping around 30 meters below the ocean surface with SCUBA
            tanks, and none for exiting from an airlock to repair a telescope in freefall.
            Although the human species is subject to natural selection, the speed with
            which we colonize unfamiliar environments shows that people do not primar-
            ily adapt via alterations in the genome. Skills are learned, not inherited. our
            species-characteristic adaptation is not any particular behavior, or repertoire
            of behaviors, but our superior ability to change our behavior.
               The cost of this survival strategy is that each new generation has to acquire
            the necessary skills anew. Human babies are born with almost no capacity for
            effective action, but a normal day in the life of, for example, a white-collar
            worker in a Western city requires an astonishing range of skills: cook break-
            fast; drive to work; use cell phones, word processors and copiers; communicate
            via e-mail and fax; transform numerical data into graphs and figures; master
            the tango or the violin; arrange a successful birthday party; care for the fish
            in the tank and keep upright on the mountain bike. Although the execution
            of an acquired skill might be quick and effortless, the acquisition of that skill
            requires both time and effort. Alone among mammals, a human being requires
            15–20 years to become a competent member of the species, and a 20-year-old
            future professional faces yet another decade of training before he has mastered
            the skills needed to excel in a complicated, typically high-technology work
            environment.
               once mastered, skills remain in flux. task environments change due to
            their own internal causal dynamics as well as in response to externalities and
   183   184   185   186   187   188   189   190   191   192   193