Page 232 - The Social Animal
P. 232

214 The Social Animal


           that it wasn’t completely a lie and the task wasn’t quite as dull as it
           seemed at first; as a matter of fact, when looked at in a certain way, it
           was actually quite interesting.
               It would be fruitful to compare these results with Judson Mills’s
                                                         35
           data on the effects of cheating among 6th-graders. Recall that, in
           Mills’s experiment, the decision about whether to cheat was almost
           certainly a difficult one for most of the children. This is why they ex-
           perienced dissonance, regardless of whether they cheated or resisted
           temptation. One could speculate about what would happen if the re-
           wards to be gained by cheating were very large. For one thing, it
           would be more tempting to cheat; therefore, more children would ac-
           tually cheat. But, more important, if the gains for cheating were as-
           tronomical, those who cheated would undergo very little attitude
           change. Much like the college students who lied in Festinger and
           Carlsmith’s $20 condition, those children who cheated for a great re-
           ward would have less need to reduce dissonance, having been pro-
           vided with an abundance of external justification for their behavior. In
           fact, Mills did include this refinement in his experiment, and his re-
           sults are consistent with this reasoning: Those who cheated to obtain
           a small reward tended to soften their attitude about cheating more
           than those who cheated to obtain a large reward. Moreover, those who
           refrained from cheating in spite of the temptation of a large reward—
           a choice that would create a great deal of dissonance—hardened their
           attitude about cheating to a greater extent than those who refrained
           in the face of a small reward—just as one might expect.

           Dissonance and the Self-Concept The analysis of the disso-
           nance phenomenon presented in this section requires a departure
           from Festinger’s original theory. In the experiment by Festinger and
           Carlsmith, for example, the original statement of dissonance went
           like this: The cognition “I believe the task is dull” is dissonant with
           the cognition “I said the task was interesting.” Several years ago, I re-
           formulated the theory in a way that focuses more attention on the
                                            36
           way people conceive of themselves. Basically, this reformulation
           suggests that dissonance is most powerful in situations in which the
           self-concept is threatened.Thus, for me, the important aspect of dis-
           sonance in the situation described above is not that the cognition “I
           said ‘X’” is dissonant with the cognition “I believe ‘not X.’” Rather,
           the crucial fact is that I have misled people: The cognition “I have
   227   228   229   230   231   232   233   234   235   236   237