Page 504 - Deep Learning
P. 504
References 487
Levine, M. W. (2000). Fundamental’s of sensation and perception (3rd ed.). Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.
Levinson, M. (2006). The box: How the shipping container made the world smaller and
the world economy bigger. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Levy, D., & Newborn, M. (1991). How computers play chess. New York: W. H.
Freeman.
Lewin, R. (1992). Complexity: Life at the edge of chaos. New York: Macmillan.
Lewis, C. (1987). Composition of productions. In D. Klahr, P. Langley, & R. Neches
(Eds.), Production system models of learning and development (pp. 329–358).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
———. (1988). Why and how to learn why: Analysis-based generalization of procedures.
Cognitive Science, vol. 12, pp. 211–256.
Liebherr, J. K., & McHugh, J. V. (2003). Coleoptera. In V. H. Resh & R. T. Cardé (Eds.),
Encyclopedia of insects (pp. 209–229). New York: Academic Press.
Limón, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual
change: A critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, vol. 11, pp. 357–380.
Limón, M., & Mason, L. (Eds.). (2002). Reconsidering conceptual change: Issues in
theory and practice. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.
Lipton, P. (2004). Inference to the best explanation (2nd ed.). London, UK: Routledge.
Little, D. M., & Thulborn, K. R. (2006). Prototype-distortion category learning: A
two-phase process across a distributed network. Brain and Cognition, vol. 60,
pp. 233–243.
Liu, S.-C., & Liu, S.-Y. (2001). An efficient expert system for air compressor
troubleshooting. Expert Systems, vol. 18, pp. 203–214.
Lockhart, R. S. (2002). Levels of processing, transfer-appropriate processing, and the
concept of robust encoding. Memory, vol. 10, pp. 397–403.
Lockman, J. J. (2000). A perception-action perspective on tool use development. Child
Development, vol. 71, pp. 137–144.
Logan, G. D. (1998). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological
Review, vol. 95, pp. 492–527.
Lombrozo, T., & Carey, S. (2006). Functional explanation and the function of
explanation. Cognition, vol. 99, pp. 167–204.
Losee, J. (2005). Theories on the scrap heap: Scientists and philosophers on the falsification,
rejection, and replacement of theories. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh
Press.
Lovejoy, A. O., & Boas, G. (1935/1997). Primitivism and related ideas in antiquity.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Lu, X., Di Eugenio, B., Kershaw, T., Ohlsson, S., & Corrigan-Halpern, A. (2007). Expert
vs. non-expert tutoring: Dialogue moves, interaction patterns and multi-utterance
turns. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol. 4394, pp. 456–467.
Lubart, T. I. (2000–2001). Models of the creative process: Past, present and future.
Creativity Research Journal, vol. 13, pp. 295–308.
Lucas, J. R. (1961). Minds, machines and Gödel. Philosophy, vol. 36, pp. 112–127.
Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung.
Psychological Monographs (Whole No. 248), vol. 54, pp. 1–95.