Page 511 - Deep Learning
P. 511

494                         References

            Nokes,  T.  J.  (2009).  Mechanisms  of  knowledge  transfer.  Thinking  and  Reasoning,
               vol. 15, pp. 1–36.
            Nokes, T. J., & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Declarative transfer from a memory task to a problem
               solving task. Cognitive Science Quarterly, vol. 3, pp. 259–296.
            Nokes, T., & Ohlsson, S. (2005). Comparing multiple paths to mastery: What is learned?
               Cognitive Science, vol. 29, pp. 769–796.
            Norman,  D.  (1981).  Categorization  of  action  slips.  Psychological  Review,  vol.  88,
               pp. 1–15.
            Norman, G. (2005). Research in clinical reasoning: Past history and current trends.
               Medical Education, vol. 39, pp. 418–427.
            Nussbaum,  J.  (1985).  The  Earth  as  a  cosmic  body.  In  R.  Driver,  E.  Guesne,  &
               A.  Tiberghien  (Eds.),  Children’s  ideas  in  science  (pp.  170–192).  Milton  Keynes,
               UK: Open University Press.
            Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (1998). Rational models of cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford
               University Press.
            O’Connor, D. J. (1975). The correspondence theory of truth. London, UK: Hutchinson.
            Ohlsson, S. (1983). The enaction theory of thinking and its educational implications.
               Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, vol. 27, pp. 73–88.
            ———. (1984a). Restructuring revisited I. Summary and critique of the Gestalt theory
               of problem solving. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, vol. 25, pp. 65–78.
            ———. (1984b). Restructuring revisited II. An information processing theory of restruc-
               turing and insight. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, vol. 25, pp. 117–129.
            ———. (1984c). Induced strategy shifts in spatial reasoning. Acta Psychologica, vol. 57,
               pp. 47–67.
            ———. (1986). Some principles of intelligent tutoring. Instructional Science, vol. 14,
               pp. 293–326.
            ———.  (1987a).  Truth  versus  appropriateness:  Relating  declarative  to  procedural
               knowledge. In D. Klahr, P. Langley, & R. Neches (Eds.), Production system models
               of learning and development (pp. 287–327). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
            ———. (1987b). Transfer of training in procedural learning: A matter of conjectures
               and refutations? In L. Bolc (Ed.), Computational models of learning (pp. 55–88).
               New York: Springer-Verlag.
            ———. (1987c). Sense and reference in the design of interactive illustrations for rational
               numbers. In R. Lawler & M. Yazdani (Eds.), Artificial Intelligence and Education
               (pp. 203–237). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
            ———.  (1988a).  Computer  simulation  and  its  impact  on  educational  research  and
               practice. International Journal of Education, vol. 12, pp. 5–34.
            ———. (1990a). Trace analysis and spatial reasoning: An example of intensive cognitive
               diagnosis and its implications for testing. In N. Frederiksen, R. Glaser, A. Lesgold,
               & M. G. Shafto (Eds.), Diagnostic monitoring of skill and knowledge acquisition
               (pp. 251–296). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
            ———. (1990b). Cognitive science and instruction: Why the revolution is not here (yet). In
               H. Mandl, E. DeCorte, N. Bennett, & H. F. Freidrich (Eds.), Learning and instruction,
               European research in an international context (vol. 2.1): Social and cognitive aspects
               of learning and instruction (pp. 561–600). New York: Pergamon Press.
   506   507   508   509   510   511   512   513   514   515   516