Page 446 - Deep Learning
P. 446

Notes to Pages 184–192                429

                represented by a dozen rules. For complex strategies, the number is at least an
                order of magnitude greater.
              24.  But see Clancey (1985) and Lenat (1983) for two exceptions, as well as the collec-
                tion of papers in Groner, Groner and Bischof (1983).
              25.  McDermott and Forgy (1978).
              26.  The term was introduced by Anderson (1983) and it has come into widespread
                use as a convenient verbal label for the innate cognitive equipment, that is, the
                totality of the cognitive structures and processes that are not acquired through
                learning. See Cooper (2006) for a philosophical discussion of this concept.
              27.  VanLehn (1991).
              28.  James (1890, vols. 1 and 2).
              29.  Thorndike (1898).
              30.  Bryan and Harter (1897, 1899). This pioneering study has had more impact on
                micro-economics than on psychology; see, e.g., Jovanovic and Nyarko (1995) and
                Reis (1991).
              31.  Ebbinghaus (1885/1964).
              32.  Watson (1913). Hilgard and Bower (1966) is the classic and most comprehensive
                review of behaviorist learning theories.
              33.  See Woodworth (1938) for the summary of complex skill acquisition (pp. 156–175)
                and for a discussion of the learning curve (pp. 170–173).
              34.  Woodworth (1938, p. 164).
              35.  Gardner (1985, Chap. 2). See also the introduction to Wiener (1948, pp. 7–39), the
                historical observations in Miller (2003) and the historical addendum in Newell
                and Simon (1972a, pp. 873–889).
              36.  Wiener (1948); see Conway and Siegelman (2005) and Heims (1991) for the his-
                tory of the cybernetic movement.
              37.  Boden (1972, Chap. 4), Miller, Galanter and Pribram (1960), Moray (1987), Powers
                (1973), Smith (1966) and Smith and Smith (1966).
              38.  Conway and Siegelman (2005).
              39.  See Crevier (1993) and Gardner (1985) for the history of cognitive science and
                Newell, Shaw and Simon (1958) for the original article.
              40.  Ausubel (1968) and Gagné (1965). See R. C. Anderson (1967) for a review of the
                period.
              41.  Fitts (1964) and Welford (1968).
              42.  Anzai and Simon (1979).
              43.  Anderson (1982, 1983, 1987) and Anderson, Kline and Beasley (1978, 1979).
              44.  See Rosenbloom, Laird and Newell (1993) and Rosenbloom and Newell (1986,
                1987). The original Soar model learned only by capturing paths to subgoals dis-
                covered during problem solving, but Nason and Laird (2005) extended the sys-
                tem with feedback-based (reinforcement) learning.
              45.  Sun, Merrill and Peterson (2001); and Sun, Slusarz and Terry (2005).
              46.  VanLehn (1999), VanLehn and Jones (1993), and VanLehn, Jones and Chi (1992).
                Cascade  grew  out  of  a  prior  model  of  learning  from  examples  in  arithmetic
                (VanLehn, 1990).
              47.  For a technical review, see Ohlsson (2008a).
              48.  Bradshaw (1992).
   441   442   443   444   445   446   447   448   449   450   451