Page 40 - The Social Animal
P. 40

22 The Social Animal


           conform, there were no explicit constraints against individuality. In
           many situations, the sanctions against nonconformity are clear and
           unequivocal. For example, I hate to wear a tie, and under most cir-
           cumstances I can get away with this minor idiosyncrasy. On occa-
           sion, however, I can’t. I often find myself stopped at the entrance
           to a restaurant and politely (but firmly) informed that if I refuse to
           don the tie offered me by the maitre d’, I cannot dine in the restau-
           rant. I can either put on the tie and eat in the restaurant or leave,
           open-necked and comfortable but hungry. The negative conse-
           quences of nonconformity are made very explicit.
               But in Asch’s experiment (and in the hypothetical example of
           Sam watching the candidate on television), the situations were much
           more subtle. In these situations, there were no explicit rewards for
           conformity and no explicit punishments for deviance. Why, then, did
           Asch’s participants and Sam conform? There are two major possibil-
           ities; either they became convinced, in the face of the judgment of
           the unanimous majority, that their own opinions were wrong, or they
           “went along with the crowd” (while inwardly believing their initial
           judgments were correct) in order to be accepted by the majority or to
           avoid being disliked by them for disagreeing.
               In short, what I am suggesting is that these individuals had two
           important goals: the goal of being correct and the goal of staying in the
           good graces of other people by living up to their expectations. In many
           circumstances, both of these goals can be satisfied by a simple action.
           Driving on the right-hand side of the road is the correct thing to do,
           and it satisfies other people’s expectations. So, too, are telephoning
           your mother on Mother’s Day, giving proper directions to a visitor in
           town, and studying hard to perform well on an exam. Similarly, if oth-
           ers agreed with your judgment of the lengths of the lines, you could
           satisfy both goals by being true to your own estimate. But, in Asch’s
           experiment, these two goals were placed in conflict. If you were a par-
           ticipant in that experiment and you initially believed that the correct
           answer was line B, then saying so might satisfy your desire to be cor-
           rect—but it might also violate the expectations of your peers, and they
           might think you a bit odd. On the other hand, choosing line A might
           win you the acceptance of the others, but unless you became convinced
           that they were correct, it would violate your desire to be right.
               Most people believe that they are motivated primarily by a de-
           sire to be correct but that others are motivated primarily by a desire
   35   36   37   38   39   40   41   42   43   44   45