Page 15 - HOW TO TEACH GRAMMAR
P. 15

We all know what involves riding a bike: keeping your balance, pedaling, steering by means of
                  the handlebars and so on. This does not mean to say that we might know how to ride a bike. The same
                  analogy applies to language learning. It can be view as a body of knowledge – such as vocabulary and
                  grammar. Or it can be viewed as a skill (or a complex set of skills). If I you take the language-is-skill
                  point of view, then it follows that, like bike riding, you learn by doing it, not by studying it. Learning-by-
                  doing is what is called experimental learning.

                  The communication argument

                         There is more to knowing a language than knowing its grammar. It is one thing to that Do you
                  drink? is a present simple question. It is another thing to know that it can function as an offer. This
                  simple observation is at heart of what is now called the Communicative approach, or Communicative
                  Language Teaching (CLT). Communicative competence involves knowing how to use the grammar
                  and vocabulary of the language to achieve communicative goals and knowing how to do this in a
                  socially appropriate way.

                         Two schools of thought emerged as to the best means of achieving the objectives of this
                  communicative approach. The first – or shallow- end approach – might be summed up the view that
                  you  learn  a  language  in  order  to  use  it.  That  is:  learn  the  rules  and  then  apply  them  in  life-like
                  communication.  The  more  radical  line,  however,  is  that  you  use  a  language  in  order  to  learn  it.
                  Proponents of this deep-end approach take an experiential view of learning: you learn to communicate
                  by  communicating.  They  argue  that,  by  means  of  activities  that  engage  the  learner  in  life-like
                  communication, the grammar will be acquired virtually unconsciously. Studying the rules of grammar
                  is simply a waste of valuable time.

                  The acquisition argument.
                         The fact that we all learn our first language without being, taught grammar has not escaped
                  theorist. Learning, according to Krashen, results from formal instruction, typically in grammar, and is of
                  limited use for real communication. Acquisition, however, is natural process: it is the process by which
                  the first language is picked up, and by which other languages are picked up solely through contact with
                  speakers of those languages. Success in a second language is due to acquisition, not learning, he
                  argues. Moreover, he claims that learned knowledge can never become acquired knowledge.
                  The natural order argument.

                         This view derives partly from the work of the linguist Noam Chomsky. Chomsky argues that
                  humans are “hard-wired” to learn languages: that is, there are universal principles of grammar that we
                  are born with. The idea of an innate universal grammar helps explain similarities in the developmental
                  order in first language acquisition as well as in second language acquisition. It explains why English
                  children, Thai teenagers and Saudi adults all go through a “I no like fish stage” before progressing to “I
                  don‟t like fish”. In short, the natural order argument insists that a textbook grammar is not, nor can ever
                  become, a mental grammar.

                  For you to read some more: https://www.britannica.com/topic/universal-grammar









                                                             15
   10   11   12   13   14   15   16   17   18   19   20