Page 102 - Essencials of Sociology
P. 102
Learning Personality, Morality, and Emotions 75
in balancing these conflicting demands of the id and the superego. In the maladjusted
individual, the ego fails to control the conflict between the id and the superego. Either
the id or the superego dominates this person, leading to internal confusion and prob-
lem behaviors.
Sociological Evaluation. Sociologists appreciate Freud’s emphasis on socialization—
his assertion that the social group into which we are born transmits norms and values
that restrain our biological drives. Sociologists, however, object to the view that inborn
and subconscious motivations are the primary reasons for human behavior. This denies
the central principle of sociology: that factors such as social class (income, education, and
occupation) and people’s roles in groups underlie their behavior (Epstein 1988; Bush
and Simmons 1990).
Feminist sociologists have been especially critical of Freud. Although what I just sum-
marized applies to both females and males, Freud assumed that “male” is “normal.” He
even referred to females as inferior, castrated males (Chodorow 1990; Gerhard 2000). It
is obvious that sociologists need to continue to research how we develop personality.
Kohlberg and the Development of Morality
If you have observed young children, you know that they want immediate gratification
and show little or no concern for others. (“Mine!” a 2-year-old will shout, as she grabs a
toy from another child.) Yet, at a later age, this same child will be considerate of others
and try to be fair in her play. How does this change happen?
Kohlberg’s Theory. Psychologist Lawrence Kohlberg (1975, 1984, 1986; Reed
2008) concluded that we go through a sequence of stages as we develop morality. Build-
ing on Piaget’s work, he found that children start in the amoral stage I just described.
For them, there is no right or wrong, just personal needs to be satisfied. From about
ages 7 to 10, children are in what Kohlberg called a preconventional stage. They have
learned rules, and they follow them to stay out of trouble. They view right and wrong
as what pleases or displeases their parents, friends, and teachers. Their concern is to get
rewards and to avoid punishment. At about age 10, they enter the conventional stage.
During this period, morality means following the norms and values they have learned.
This is followed by a postconventional stage in which individuals reflect on abstract prin-
ciples of right and wrong and judge people’s behavior according to these principles.
Criticisms of Kohlberg. Carol Gilligan, another psychologist, was one of the first to
criticize Kohlberg. She noticed that Kohlberg had studied only boys. When she inter-
viewed men and women, she concluded that women are more likely to evaluate morality
in terms of personal relationships—how an act affects others and the harm it might bring
to loved ones. Other researchers followed up, finding that both men and women use
personal relationships and abstract principles when they make moral judgments (Wark
and Krebs 1996).
To test Kohlberg’s theory, researchers checked how it applies in different cultures.
They found that the preconventional and conventional stages apply around the world.
Most societies, though, do not have the postconventional stage of universal reasoning.
This stage appears to be mostly a Western concept (Jensen 2009). Apparently, there is
no universal, abstract way of figuring what is moral. Instead, different cultures have their
own ways to determine morality, and each teaches its members to use its norms in decid-
ing what is moral.
Research with Babies. Researchers have developed ingenious experiments to see if
babies have a morality (Bloom 2010; Hamlin and Wynn 2011). In one experiment, they
showed babies a puppet that helps another puppet and one that interferes with that pup-
pet. They found that babies—even under 1 year of age—prefer the “good” puppet and
want the “bad” puppet punished. From these experiments, some draw the intriguing
conclusion that we are born with a basic morality and a desire to punish those who break
our moral codes. Others suggest that the experiments are flawed (Scarf et al. 2012).
More research should eventually settle the question.