Page 470 - Deep Learning
P. 470

Notes to Pages 369–386                453

              15.  Barber and Lichtman (1999), Chechik, Meilijson and Ruppin (1998), Hua and
                Smith (2004), Huttenlocher, de Courten, Garey and van der Loos (1982) and
                Purves and Lichtman (1980).
              16.  Zuckerman and Cohen (1964).
              17.  For a review of this concept from a linguistic point of view, see Carnie (2008).
                The arguments for why and how this concept applies to the language of thought
                were reviewed by Fodor (1976) and Fodor and Pylyshyn (1988). Briefly put, “the
                constituency relation is a part/whole relation: If C is a constituent of C*, then a
                token of C is a part of every token of C*” (Fodor, 1997, p. 111).
              18.  See references in Chapter 4, Notes 19 and 21.
              19.  Gaskell  (2007),  Graesser,  Gernsbacher  and  Goldman  (2003)  and  Traxler  and
                Gernsbacher (2006).
              20.  Ohlsson (2008a) and Proctor and Dutta (1995).
              21.  Chi and Ohlsson (2005).
              22.  The number 10 million is mentioned on the Web site of the Los Alamos National
                Laboratory, at http://periodic.lanl.gov/elements/6.html. They in turn refer to the
                online source CRC Handbook of Chemistry and Physics. The exact number does
                not matter for present purposes, only its order of magnitude.
              23.  Liebherr and McHugh (2003).
              24.  Thorndike (1932): “Punishments often but not always tend to shift from [the con-
                nections that lead to them] to something else. … They weaken the connection
                which produced them, when they do weaken it, by strengthening some compet-
                ing connection” (p. 277).
              25.  Flight and vision are the two classical challenges of this sort. For the two-stage
                theory of the evolution of flight in birds, see Dial (2003) and Sumida and Brochut
                (2000). For a similarly structured theory of flight in insects, see Kingsolver and
                Koehl (1985). For a discussion of the evolution of vision, see Gregory (2008).
                Budd (2006, especially pp. 618–619) and Gregory (2008, especially p. 361) pro-
                pose general principles, including functional shifts and multiple stages with dif-
                ferent selective pressures, for the evolution of complex adaptations.
              26.  The term “future shock” was introduced by Alvin Tofffler in the book with that
                title  (Toffler,  1970).  His  thesis  was  that  rapid  change  imposes  a  strain  on  the
                human mind. Bertman (1998) followed up on this thesis 25 years later. The pre-
                dicted collapse of the human psyche due to too much change appears not to be
                forthcoming.
              27.  See Mansfield, Busse and Krepelka (1978) and Scott, Leritz and Mumford (2004).
                The evidence is not much stronger for training programs that claim to raise a
                person’s intelligence as measured by IQ-tests (Perkins, 1995b).
              28.  Picasso is often quoted as saying, “I don’t search, I find.” What he said was, “In my
                opinion to search means nothing in painting. To find, is the thing.” The comment,
                presumably spoken in either French or Spanish, was recorded by Marius de Zayas,
                translated into English and published in an article titled “Picasso Speaks” in the
                New York magazine The Arts, May 1923. (Reprinted in Ashton, 1972, pp. 3–6.)
              29.  Louis  Pasteur,  in  his  inaugural  lecture  as  professor  and  dean,  University  of
                Lille,  Douai,  France,  December  7,  1854.  English  translation  in  Peterson  (1954,
                pp. 469–474).
   465   466   467   468   469   470   471   472   473   474   475