Page 475 - Deep Learning
P. 475

458                          References

           ———.  (2002).  Spanning  seven  orders  of  magnitude:  A  challenge  for  cognitive
               modeling. Cognitive Science, vol. 26, pp. 85–112.
           ———. (2004). Cognitive psychology and its implications (6th ed.). New York: Worth.
           ———. (2005). Human symbol manipulation within an integrated cognitive architecture.
               Cognitive Science, vol. 29, pp. 313–341.
           ———.  (2007).  How  can  the  human  mind  occur  in  the  physical  universe?  New
               York: Oxford University Press.
           Anderson,  J.  R.,  &  Bower,  G.  H.  (1973).  Human  associative  memory.  Washington,
               DC: Winston.
           Anderson,  J.  R.,  &  Jeffries,  R.  (1985).  Novice  LISP  errors:  Undetected  losses  of
               information  from  working  memory.  Human-Computer  Interaction,  vol.  1,
               pp. 107–131.
           Anderson, J. R., Kline, P., & Beasley, C. M. (1978, February). A theory of the acquisition
               of cognitive skills (Technical Report 77–1). New Haven, CT: Yale University.
           Anderson, J. R., Kline, P. J., & Beasley, C. M. (1979). A general learning theory and
               its application to schema abstraction. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of
               learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (vol. 13, pp. 277–318).
               New York: Academic Press.
           Anderson, J. R., Kushmerick, N., & Lebiere, C. (1993). The Tower of Hanoi and goal
               structures. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Rules of the mind (pp. 121–142). Hillsdale, NJ:
               Erlbaum.
           Anderson,  J.  R.,  &  Lebiere,  C.  (Eds.).  (1998).  The  atomic  components  of  thought.
               Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
           Anderson, J. R., & Pirolli, P. L. (1984). Spread of activation. Journal of Experimental
               Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 10, pp. 791–798.
           Anderson, J. R., & Schooler, L. J. (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory.
               Psychological Science, vol. 2, pp. 396–408.
           ———. (2000). The adaptive nature of memory. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The
               Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 557–570). New York: Oxford University Press.
           Anderson,  M.  L.  (2003).  Embodied  cognition:  A  field  guide.  Artificial  Intelligence,
               vol. 149, pp. 91–130.
           Anderson, R. C. (1967). Educational psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 18,
               pp. 129–164.
           Anderson, S. R. (2004). Doctor Dolittle’s delusion: Animals and the uniqueness of human
               language. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
           Anzai, Y., & Simon, H. A. (1979). The theory of learning by doing. Psychological Review,
               vol. 86, pp. 124–140.
           Arnauld, A., & Nicole, P. (1662/1996). Logic or the art of thinking: Containing, besides
               common  rules,  several  new  observations  appropriate  for  forming  judgments
               (J. V. Buroker, Trans.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
           Aronson, E. (1997). Back to the future: Retrospective review of Leon Festinger’s “A theory
               of cognitive dissonance.” American Journal of Psychology, vol. 110, pp. 127–137.
           Ash, M. G. (1995). Gestalt psychology in German culture, 1890–1967: Holism and the
               quest for objectivity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
           Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (2005). Human category learning. Annual Review of
               Psychology, vol. 56, pp. 149–178.
   470   471   472   473   474   475   476   477   478   479   480