Page 380 - The Social Animal
P. 380

362 The Social Animal


           knows it. The boss comes along and uses the same phrase in exactly
           the same tone of voice. Will that phrase function as a reward in this
           situation? I am not sure. Nancy may interpret the statement as her
           boss’s attempt to be encouraging and nice, even in the face of a poor
           performance; because of the boss’s display of thoughtfulness, Nancy
           may come to like him even more than she would have had she, in
           fact, done a good job. On the other hand, Nancy may attribute all
           kinds of characteristics or ulterior motives to her boss: She may con-
           clude that her boss is being sarcastic, manipulative, dishonest,
           nondiscriminating, patronizing, seductive, or stupid—any one of
           which could reduce Nancy’s liking for him. A general reward-cost
           theory loses a good deal of its value if our definition of what consti-
           tutes a reward is not clear. As situations become complex, we find
           that such general notions decrease in value because a slight change
           in the social context in which the reward is provided can change a
           reward into a punishment.
               Research in this area indicates that, although people like to be
                                           13
           praised and tend to like the praiser, they also dislike being manip-
           ulated. If the praise is too lavish, it seems unwarranted, or (most im-
           portant) if the praiser is in a position to benefit from the ingratiating
           behavior, then he or she is not liked very much. In an experiment by
           Edward Jones, an accomplice observed a young woman being inter-
           viewed and then proceeded to evaluate her. The evaluations were
                                                  14
           prearranged so that some women heard a positive evaluation, some
           heard a negative evaluation, and some heard a neutral evaluation. In
           one experimental condition, the evaluator might have had an ulterior
           motive. In this condition, participants were informed in advance that
           the evaluator was a graduate student who needed participants for her
           own experiment and would be asking the students to volunteer. The
           results showed that the students liked the evaluators who praised
           them better than those who provided them with a negative evalua-
           tion, but there was a sharp drop in their liking for the praiser with
           the possible ulterior motive. Thus the old adage “flattery will get you
           nowhere” is clearly wrong. As Jones put it, “flattery will get you some-
           where”—but not everywhere.
               By the same token, we like people who do us favors. Favors can
           be considered rewards, and we tend to like people who provide us
           with this kind of reward. For example, in a classic study of inmates
           in a women’s reformatory, Helen Hall Jennings found that the most
   375   376   377   378   379   380   381   382   383   384   385