Page 9 - Testing UTS
P. 9
428 Markus and Kitayama
Concluding Remarks References
In the last three decades of examining culture and self, most Adams, G. (2005). The cultural grounding of personal relationship:
cultural psychological studies have been done by middle- Enemyship in North American and West African worlds. Journal
class North Americans, and they have used middle-class North of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 948–968.
Americans as one of the major comparison groups. The upside Adams, G., & Markus, H.R. (2004). Toward a conception of culture
of this Euro-American centrism is that psychologists have suitable for a social psychology of culture. In M. Schaller &
learned a great deal about one particular category of C.S. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture
humans—those from middle-class North American and West- (pp. 335–360). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ern European contexts (Arnett, 2008; Markus, Kitayama, & Adams, G. (2005). The cultural grounding of personal relationship:
Heiman, 1997). People engaging in these contexts are likely Enemyship in North American and West African worlds. Journal
to reveal relatively high levels of self-esteem, self-efficacy, of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 948–968.
optimism, or intrinsic motivation and express a desire for mas- Arnett, J.J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology
tery, control, achievement, choice, self-expression, or unique- needs to become less American. American Psychologist, 63,
ness. They also like to feel happy, upbeat, and successful, 602–614.
and their agency often takes the form of influencing others or Atran, S., Medin, D.L., & Ross, N.O. (2005). The cultural mind: Envi-
the world. We can now confidently say that this robust set of ronmental decision making and cultural modeling within and
psychological tendencies—with its many world-making and across populations. Psychological Review, 112, 744–776.
world-maintaining consequences—is not, however, an Banaji, M., & Prentice, D. (1994). The self in social contexts. Annual
expression of a universal human nature. Instead, it reflects the Review of Psychology, 45, 297–332.
particular worlds in which these people engage. These well- Brewer, M.B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this ‘‘we"? Levels of
documented self-serving and self-interested tendencies are cre- collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality
ated, fostered, and maintained by widely distributed ideas, such and Social Psychology, 71, 83–93.
as the importance of individual achievement, that have been Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
reinforced and instituted by dense networks of everyday prac- University Press.
tices, such as complimenting and praising one another for indi- Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1998). On the self-regulation of beha-
vidual performance, frequently distributing awards and honors vior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
in classrooms and workplaces, and promoting the self in situa- Cohen, A.B., & Rozin, P. (2001). Religion and the morality of mental-
tions like applying for jobs. These tendencies for self- ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 697–710.
expression, feeling good about the self, and controlling the Cohen, D. (2007). Methods in cultural psychology. In S. Kitayama &
environment are further encouraged by products such as coffee D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 196–236).
mugs, bumper stickers, self-help books, automobile advertise- New York: Guilford.
ments, medications, perfume, and cleaning products that exhort Cohen, D., & Hoshino-Browne, E. (2005). Insider and outsider per-
people to ‘‘Be a star,’’ ‘‘Take control,’’ ‘‘Never follow,’’ and spectives on the self and social world. In R.M. Sorrentino,
‘‘Get happy.’’ Notably, when people inhabit many other kinds D. Cohen, J.M. Olson & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), Culture and social
of worlds that are configured with ideas, practices, and institu- behavior: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 10, pp. 49–76).
tions that do not construct the self as the primary source of Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
action, strikingly different psychological tendencies are Cousins, S.D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the
revealed. United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
Although a vast amount of both theoretical and empirical 124–131.
work remains before researchers can more fully specify the D’Andrade, R.G. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology.
cycles of mutual constitution between cultures and selves, this Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
work is steadily changing the way psychology understands the Dumont, L. (1977). From Mandeville to Marx: The genesis and tri-
person. Psychologists and all behavioral scientists are less cer- umph of economic ideology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
tain about what can be designated as basic or universal psycho- Fiske, A., Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., & Nisbett, R.E. (1998). The
logical process and more certain that it is not possible to cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, &
develop a comprehensive human psychology by focusing G. Lindzey, The handbook of social psychology, vol. 2 (4th ed.,
solely on the individual and on what is inside that individual pp. 915–981). San Francisco: McGraw-Hill.
(e.g., Barrett, Mesquita, & Smith, in press; Bruner, 1990). Such Fiske, S.T., & Taylor, S.E. (1994). Social cognition. (2nd ed.).
a psychology will require a focus on humans’ remarkable Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
capacity to create cultures and then to be shaped by them. Gelfand, M.J., Nishii, L.H., & Raver, J.L. (2006). On the nature and
importance of cultural tightness-looseness. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 91, 1225–1244.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Greenfield, P.M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect change: Shifting pathways of human development. Developmental
to their authorship or the publication of this article. Psychology, 45, 401–418.
428