Page 57 - Zoo Animal Learning and Training
P. 57

2.8  A Final Reminder: Using Different Approaches  29

  VetBooks.ir  a prominent feature of our own lives that it is   reared birds had to learn to avoid them
                                                      (Exnerová et al. 2007). Bumble bees (Bombus
             easy to take learning for granted, but we can
             also often assume that much of what ani-
                                                      of certain colours, but learn new preferences
             mals do is in some way innate, and does not   terrestris) have innate preferences for flowers
             require learning. The field of animal behav-  through reinforced exposure to different
             iour has long been influenced by two differ-  colours (Gumbert 2000).
             ent traditions; on one hand, comparative   All of this, of course, has important implica-
             psychology, promoted the view that most   tions for the way we manage animals in zoos.
             behaviour was learned, primarily through   The aim to maintain captive animals with all
             conditioning, and that these processes were   of their species‐appropriate behaviours must
             best studied in the laboratory, whereas ethol-  clearly be not just to prevent the loss of
             ogy (and more recently, behavioural ecology)   behaviour through inbreeding or inadvertent
             promoted the view that most behaviour was   genetic  selection  (i.e. ‘domestication’),  but
             innate, the result of evolutionary processes,   also to ensure that they learn about as many
             and that these processes were best studied in   as possible  of the things that those species
             the animals’ natural habitat. More recently,   would learn about in the wild. These can
             the two approaches have started to come   range from learning about species‐identity
             together, with more demonstrations of ani-  (e.g. ensuring that hand‐reared birds do not
             mals’ learning abilities that have been observed   imprint on the wrong species) to acquiring
             in the wild, and how this influences their   food‐handling and predator‐avoidance skills
             evolution (Dukas 2004; Shettleworth 2001).  (particularly if the animals are destined for
               We know today that certain sequences   reintroduction to the wild). We know from
             of  behaviours are provided by genetic pro-  our domesticated species that behaviours do
             cesses. These sometimes exhibit variability   change in comparison with the non‐domesti-
             and the behaviours that are expressed, can be   cated forms, and this can affect learning. For
             changed through developmental processes   example,  domesticated guinea pigs  (Cavia
             and learning during the lifetime of the animal.   porcellus) are less bold and aggressive than
             Hatchery‐reared salmon (Salmo salar) show   wild cavies, but are able to learn associations
             a greater response to predator odours at   faster (Brust and Guenther 2014). Bengalese
             10–15 weeks of age than at 26–36 weeks, and   finches were domesticated from the white‐
             this recognition of the odour is innate. But   rumped munia (Lonchura striata) on criteria
             they have a peak of learning about predator   related  to  good  parenting  ability,  but  their
             odours at an age of 16–20 weeks, when they   song learning has been affected too, with the
             would, if in the wild, change habitats, and   munias showing a much more accurate
             this learning doesn’t occur in the hatchery,   learning of song from than the finches
             leading to a decline in responsiveness of   (Takahasi and Okanoya 2010). Hatchery‐
             older fish (Hawkins et  al. 2008). Squirrel   reared trout (Salmo trutta) show faster learn-
             monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) show an intense   ing than wild trout when foraging on cryptic
             fear of snakes if they are wild‐born or labora-  prey (Adriaenssens and Johnsson 2011).
             tory‐born, but not if they are laboratory‐born   Long-term captivity can therefore influence
             but not fed on insects, suggesting that expe-  what the animals learn, but also their ability
             rience of insects sensitises the monkeys to   to learn, sometimes in unexpected ways.
             fear of snakes (Masataka 1993). Wild‐caught   Similar things appear to be true for animals
             and naive hand‐reared blue tits and coal tits   that are not domesticated, living in long‐term
             avoided and did not attack aposematic    captivity. Captive spotted hyaenas (Crocuta
             firebugs (which were novel to all the birds),   crocuta), for example, were more successful
             indicating innate recognition, whereas in   at solving a novel problem and showed a
             the related great tits and crested tits the wild‐  greater diversity in their exploratory behav-
             caught birds avoided the bugs but the hand‐  iours when first interacting with the problem
   52   53   54   55   56   57   58   59   60   61   62