Page 61 - Zoo Animal Learning and Training
P. 61
References 33
VetBooks.ir alarm calls: speaking the same language or Schneider, K.J. (1984). Dominance, predation,
and optimal foraging in White‐Throated
learning another? Proceedings of the Royal
sparrow flocks. Ecology 65 (6): 1820.
Society of London, Series B: Biological
Sciences 276: 769–774. Seyfarth, R.M. and Cheney, D.L. (1986). Vocal
Masataka, N. (1993). Effects of experience with development in vervet monkeys. Animal
live insects on the development of fear of Behaviour 34: 1640–1658.
snakes in squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus. Shettleworth, S.J. (2001). Animal cognition
Animal Behaviour 46: 741–746. and animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour 61:
Mateo, J.M. (1996). The development of 277–286.
alarm‐call response behaviour in free‐living Sih, A. and Christensen, B. (2001). Optimal
juvenile Belding’s ground squirrel. Animal diet theory: when does it work, and when
Behaviour 52: 489–505. and why does it fail? Animal Behaviour 61
Mateo, J.M. and Holmes, W.G. (1997). (2): 379–390.
Development of alarm‐call responses in Sousa, C., Biro, D., and Matsuzawa, T. (2009).
Belding’s ground squirrels: the role of dams. Leaf‐tool use for drinking water by wild
Animal Behaviour 54: 509–524. chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): acquisition
Menzel, R. and Giurfa, M. (2001). Cognitive patterns and handedness. Animal Cognition
architecture of a mini‐brain: the 12 (1): 115–125.
honeybee. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: St Amant, R. and Horton, T.E. (2008).
62–71. Revisiting the definition of animal tool use.
Menzel, R. and Müller, U. (1996). Learning and Animal Behaviour 75 (4): 1199–1208.
memory in honeybees: from behaviour to Stryjek, R., Mioduszewska, B., Spaltabaka‐
neural substrates. Annual Review of Gędek, E., and Juszczak, G.R. (2018).
Neuroscience 19: 379–404. Wild Norway rats do not avoid predator
Much, R.M., Breck, S.W., Lance, N.J., and scents when collecting food in a familiar
Callahan, P. (2018). An ounce of prevention: habitat: a field study. Scientific Reports 8
Quantifying the effects of non‐lethal tools (1): 9475.
on wolf behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Suzuki, S., Kuroda, S., and Nishihara, T. (1995).
Science 203: 73–80. Tool‐set for termite‐fishing by chimpanzees
Nowell, A.A. and Fletcher, A.W. (2008). The in the Ndoki Forest, Congo. Behaviour 132
development of feeding behaviour in wild (3–4): 219–235.
western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla Svádová, K., Exnerová, A., Štys, P. et al. (2009).
gorilla). Behaviour 145: 171–193. Role of different colours of aposematic
Ottoni, E.B. and Mannu, M. (2001). Semifree‐ insects in learning, memory and
ranging Tufted Capuchins (Cebus apella) generalization of naïve bird predators.
spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts. Animal Behaviour 77: 327–336.
International Journal of Primatology 22 (3): Takahasi, M. and Okanoya, K. (2010). Song
347–358. learning in wild and domesticated strains of
Owings, D.H. and Leger, D.W. (1980). Chatter white‐rumped munia, Lonchura striata,
vocalizations of California ground squirrels: compared by cross‐fostering procedures:
predator‐and social‐role specificity. domestication increases song variability by
Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 54 (2): decreasing strain‐specific bias. Ethology 116:
163–184. 396–405.
Ropert‐Coudert, Y., Kato, A., Wilson, R.P., Thornton, A. and McAuliffe, K. (2006).
and Cannell, B. (2006). Foraging strategies Teaching in wild meerkats. Science 313:
and prey encounter rate of free‐ranging 227–229.
Little Penguins. Marine Biology 149 (2): Tierney, A.J. (1986). The evolution of learned
139–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/ and innate behaviour: contributions from
s00227‐005‐0188‐x. genetics and neurobiology to a theory of