Page 399 - Deep Learning
P. 399

382                         Conclusion

            range of different cognitive phenomena and they are grounded in prior empir­
            ical research. The explanations offered by the micro­theories may or may not
            be true, but they are complete in the sense of showing how certain subtypes
            of non­monotonic change can be understood as the emergent result of the
            interactions among processes that are not themselves non­monotonic change
            processes.
               This observation suggests a particular type of evolutionary explanation: the
            emergence of new functionality as a side effect of changes in prior structures.
            This type of explanation is familiar in evolutionary theory, in part due to some
            long­standing challenges to that theory: What good is half a wing or one­third
            of an eye? natural selection is blind to purpose and can only “select” among
            phenotypes that already exist; it cannot select “for” a function that will be use­
            ful to future generations, once that function has grown to sufficient power or
            versatility. so what drives change during the initial phase of the emergence of
                            25
            a novel adaptation?  A common theoretical move for such cases is to assume
            distinct evolutionary phases in which the emerging adaptation fulfills different
            functions and hence is subject to different selective pressures. To return once
            again to the evolution of flight, half a wing might be quite useful as an enlarger
            of apparent body size, jump stabilizer or thermoregulator. selective pressure
            might increase the size of the proto­wing to better fulfill those functions. once
            those pressures have provided two­thirds of a wing, the possibilities of, for
            example, long jumps and glides create a new set of selective pressures that
            shape the emerging wing to better support flight.
               A similar two­stage scenario might have played out with respect to the
            human capability for cognitive change. The unique evolutionary pathway of
            human beings – the reliance on learned instead of innate behavior patterns to
            get through the day – created selective pressures in the direction of increased
            cognitive ability. if you are going to live by your wits instead of your claws, those
            wits had better be good. As a result, the different cognitive functions – percep­
            tion, memory, problem solving, planning and plan execution – increased in
            complexity and power. As a side effect of this increase in processing power, the
            mechanisms for routine processing and monotonic learning came to exhibit
            all the properties specified by the principles in Table 11.2. At that point, non­
            monotonic change became a possibility. The survival advantages associated with
            being able to override the imperatives of prior experience in turbulent environ­
            ments brought new selective pressures to bear that pushed the cognitive system
            in a new direction. The resulting capability enabled Homo sapiens to survive
            even in environments that changed too quickly and too chaotically for species
            that rely on innate skills or the monotonic projection of prior experience.
   394   395   396   397   398   399   400   401   402   403   404