Page 519 - Deep Learning
P. 519

502                          References

           Reed,  S.  (1993).  A  schema-based  theory  of  transfer.  In  D.  K.  Detterman  &  R.  J.
               Sternberg  (Eds.),  Transfer  on  trial:  Intelligence,  cognition,  and  instruction
               (pp. 39–67). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
           Reigeluth,  C.  M.,  &  Stein,  F.  S.  (1983).  The  Elaboration  Theory  of  instruction.  In
               C. M. Reigeluth (Ed.), Instructional-design theories and models: An overview of
               their current status (pp. 335–381). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
           Reiner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Naïve physics reasoning:
               A  commitment  to  substance-based  conceptions.  Cognition  and  Instruction,
               vol. 18, pp. 1–34.
           Reis, D. A. (1991). Learning curves in food services. Journal of the Operational Research
               Society, vol. 42, pp. 623–629.
           Reisberg,  D.  (2006).  Cognition:  Exploring  the  science  of  the  mind  (3rd  ed.).  New
               York: Norton.
           Reisch,  G.  A.  (1994).  Planning  science:  Otto  Neurath  and  the  “International
               Encyclopedia of Unified Science.” British Journal for the History of the Sciences,
               vol. 27, pp. 153–175.
           Reiter,  R.  (1980).  A  logic  for  default  reasoning.  Artificial  Intelligence,  vol.  13,
               pp. 81–132.
           Renkl, A., Mandl, H., & Gruber, H. (1996). Inert knowledge: Analyses and remedies.
               Educational Psychologist, vol. 31, pp. 115–121.
           Revlin,  R.,  &  Leirer,  V.  O.  (1978).  The  effects  of  personal  biases  on  syllogistic
               reasoning: Rational decision from personalized representations. In R. Revlin &
               R. E. Mayer (Eds.), Human reasoning (pp. 51–81). New York: Wiley.
           Rhodes, R. (1989). The making of the atomic bomb. New York: Simon & Schuster.
           Richardson, A. (1969). Mental imagery. London, UK: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
           Richardson,  J.  T.  E.  (1978).  Mental  imagery  and  the  distinction  between  primary
               and secondary memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 30,
               pp. 471–485.
           Riedweg, C. (2005). Pythagoras: His life, teaching and influence (Cornell University,
               Trans.). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
           Rips,  L.  J.  (1994).  The  psychology  of  proof:  Deductive  reasoning  in  human  thinking.
               Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
           Roberts, R. M. (1989). Serendipity: Accidental discoveries in science. New York: Wiley.
           Robin, N., & Ohlsson, S. (1989) Impetus then and now: A detailed comparison between
               Jean Buridan and a single contemporary subject. In D. E. Herget (Ed.), Proceedings
               of the First International Conference on the History and Philosophy of Science in
               Science Teaching, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, November 6–10
               (pp. 292–305). Science Education and Department of Philosophy, Florida State
               University.
           Rock, D. (2006). Quiet leadership: Help people think better – don’t tell them what to do!
               New York: HarperCollins.
           Rogers, D. E. (2001). Embracing the range. In K. H. Pfenninger & V. R. Shubik (Eds.),
               The origins of creativity (pp. 47–57). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
           Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books.
           ———. (1970). Beliefs, attitudes, and values: A theory of organization and change. San
               Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
   514   515   516   517   518   519   520   521   522   523   524