Page 522 - Deep Learning
P. 522

References                       505

            Shafir, E., & LeBoeuf, R. A. (2002). Rationality. Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 53,
               pp. 491–517.
            Shanahan, M., & Baars, B. (2005). Applying global workspace theory to the frame
               problem. Cognition, vol. 98, pp. 157–176.
            Shapin, S. (1998). The scientific revolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
            Shiffrin,  R.  M.  (1970).  Memory  search.  In  D.  A.  Norman  (Ed.),  Models  of  human
               memory (pp. 375–447). New York: Academic Press.
            Shiffrin, R. M., & Nosofsky, R. M. (1994). Seven plus or minus two: A commentary on
               capacity limitations. Psychological Review, vol. 101, pp. 357–361.
            Shrager, J., & Siegler, R. S. (1998). A model of children’s strategy choices and strategy
               discoveries. Psychological Science, vol. 9, pp. 405–410.
            Shreeve, J. (1995). The Neandertal enigma: Solving the mystery of modern human origins.
               New York: William Morrow.
            Siegler, R. S. (1986). Children’s thinking. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
            ———. (1987). The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children’s
               addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol. 116, pp. 250–264.
            ———. (1989). Hazards of mental chronometry: An example from children’s subtraction.
               Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 81, pp. 497–506.
            ———.  (1996).  Emerging  minds:  The  process  of  change  in  children’s  thinking.  New
               York: Oxford University Press.
            Siegler, R., & Araya, R. (2005). A computational model of conscious and unconscious
               strategy discovery. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior
               (vol. 33, pp. 1–42). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
            Siegler,  R.  S.,  &  Crowley,  K.  (1991).  The  microgenetic  method:  A  direct  means  for
               studying cognitive development. American Psychologist, vol. 46, pp. 606–620.
            Siegler,  R.  S.,  &  Jenkins,  E.  (1989).  How  children  discover  new  strategies.  Hillsdale,
               NJ: Erlbaum.
            Silvi, B., & Savin, A. (1994). Classification of chemical bonds based on topological
               analysis of electron localization functions. Nature, vol. 371, pp. 683–686.
            Simon.  H.  A.  (1962).  The  architecture  of  complexity.  Proceedings  of  the  American
               Philosophical Society, vol. 106, pp. 467–482. [Reprinted in Simon, H. A. (1970).
               The sciences of the artificial (pp. 84–118). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.]
            ———.  (1966).  Scientific  discovery  and  the  psychology  of  problem  solving.  In
               R.  G.  Colodny  (Ed.),  Mind  and  cosmos:  Essays  in  contemporary  science  and
               philosophy (pp. 22–40). Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.
            ———. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 41, pp. 1–19.
            Simon, H. A., & Barenfeld, M. (1969). Information-processing analysis of perceptual
               processes in problem solving. Psychological Review, vol. 76, pp. 473–483.
            Simon, H. A., & Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions.
               Cognitive Psychology, vol. 5, pp. 29–46.
            Simon, H. A., & Hayes, J. R. (1976). The understanding process: Problem isomorphs.
               Cognitive Psychology, vol. 8, pp. 169–190.
            Simon, H. A., & Newell, A. (1971). Human problem solving: The state of the theory in
               1970. American Psychologist, vol. 26, pp. 145–159.
            Simonton,  D.  K.  (1988).  Scientific  genius:  A  psychology  of  science.  Cambridge,  UK:
               Cambridge University Press.
   517   518   519   520   521   522   523   524   525   526   527