Page 526 - Deep Learning
P. 526

References                       509

           Tattersall, I., & Schwartz, J. H. (2000). Extinct humans. Boulder, CO: Westview.
           Tavris,  C.,  &  Aronson,  E.  (2007).  Mistakes  were  made  (but  not  by  me).  Orlando,
               FL: Harcourt.
           Thagard, P. (1990). The conceptual structure of the chemical revolution. Philosophy of
               Science, vol. 57, pp. 183–209.
           ———. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
           ———. (2007). Abductive inference: From philosophical analysis to neural mechanisms.
               In A. Feeney & E. Heit (Eds.), Inductive reasoning: Cognitive, mathematical, and
               neuroscientific approaches (pp. 226–247). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
               Press.
           Thagard,  P.,  &  Shelley,  C.  (1997).  Abductive  reasoning:  Logic,  visual  thinking,
               and  coherence.  In  M.-L.  Dalla  Chiara,  K.  Doets,  D.  Mundici,  &  J.  van
               Benthem  (Eds.),  Logic  and  scientific  methods  (pp.  413–427).  Dordrecht,  the
               Netherlands: Kluwer.
           Thagard, P., & Verbeurgt, K. (1998). Coherence as constraint satisfaction. Cognitive
               Science, vol. 22, pp. 1–24.
           Thayer, H. S. (Ed.). (1982). Pragmatism: The classic writings. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett
               Publishing.
           Thomason,  N.  (1994).  The  power  of  ARCHED  hypotheses:  Feyerabend’s  Galileo
               as  a  closet  rationalist.  British  Journal  for  the  Philosophy  of  Science,  vol.  45,
               pp. 255–264.
           Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative
               processes in animals. PhD dissertation, New York: Teachers College, Columbia
               University.
           ———. (1927). The Law of Effect. American Journal of Psychology, vol. 39, pp. 212–222.
           ———. (1932). The fundamentals of learning. New York: Teachers College, Columbia
               University.
           Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one
               mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. (I). Psychological Review,
               vol. 8, pp. 247–261.
           Thorndyke,  P.  W.  (1984).  Applications  of  schema  theory  in  cognitive  research.  In
               J.  R.  Anderson  &  S.  M.  Kosslyn  (Eds.),  Tutorials  in  learning  and  memory
               (pp. 167–191). San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
           Timmerman,  M.  E.,  &  Brouwer,  W.  H.  (1999).  Slow  information  processing  after
               very  severe  head  injury:  Impaired  access  to  declarative  knowledge  and  intact
               application and acquisition of procedural knowledge. Neuropsychologica, vol. 37,
               pp. 467–478.
           Tishman, S., Jay, E., & Perkins, D. N. (1993). Teaching thinking dispositions: From
               transmission to enculturation. Theory into Practice, vol. 32, pp. 147–153.
           Titchener, E. B. (1912). The schema of introspection. American Journal of Psychology,
               vol. 23, pp. 485–508.
           Toffler, A. (1970). Future shock. New York: Random House.
           Tomasello,  M.  (1999a).  The  human  adaptation  for  culture.  Annual  Review  of
               Anthropology, vol. 28, pp. 509–529.
           ———.  (1999b).  The  cultural  origins  of  human  cognition.  Cambridge,  MA:  Harvard
               University Press.
   521   522   523   524   525   526   527   528   529   530   531