Page 37 - The Art of Learning by Josh Waitzkin_Neat plip book
P. 37

Children  who  begin  their  chess  educat ion  by  memorizing  open ings   tend  to
                internalize   an   entity   theory   of   intelligence.   Thei r   dialogues    with   teache rs,

                parents,   and   other   children   are   all   abo ut    resul ts,   not   effort.   The y   cons ide r
                themselves  winners  because  so  far  they   hav e  won.  In  school,  they   focus   on  wha t
                comes easy to them and ignore the subj ects that  are harder.  On  the  pl aygr ound,
                they use the famous “I wasn’t trying” af ter missing a s ho t or striki ng o ut .

                    Once  I  was  in  Arizona  giving  a  lectur e  and   simul taneous   exhi bi tion I   to  a
                large  group  of  young  chess  players  and  par ents,  and  the  organ izer  of  the   event
                picked  me  up  at  the  airport  braggi ng  that   hi s  son  had n’t  lost  a  che ss  ga me  in
                over  a  year.  Obviously  this  was  a  record  the  who le  family  was  proud  of.  I  kne w

                what  was  coming—classic  anorexic  hermit  crab.  When   I  met  the  chi ld,  he   was
                a moderately talented boy who was the  best in  hi s school.  He had learne d  some
                quick  opening  attacks  and  had  a  natur al  feel  for  bas ic  ches s  tactics.  Clearly  he
                had  started  winning  and  had  been  praised  effusively  for  hi s  gen ius .  As  a  resul t,

                the  boy  refused  to  play  anyone  outside  of  the  circle  of  friends   and  compe titors
                whom  he  knew  to  be  inferior  (his  favorite  oppo nen t  was  his  fathe r,  who   was  a
                weak player and no challenge at all). To his school buddies, this boy was a chess
                god, but compared to serious chess-pl aying  chi ldr en around  the  count ry, he  ha d

                a  long  way  to  go.  He  was  a  big  fish  in  a  small  po nd  and  he  liked   it  tha t  way.
                The   boy   avoided   chess   throughout   my   visit.   He   di dn’t   want   to   pl ay   in   the
                simultaneous  exhibition  and  was  the  onl y  chi ld  at  the  event  who   was  resistant
                to   instruction.   His   winning   streak   and   the   cons tant   talk   of   it   ha d   hi m   all

                locked  up—he  was  terrified  of  shattering      the   façade  of  perfection.   Thi s  chi ld
                was paralyzed by an ever-deepening cy cle of entity indo ctrinat ion.
                    Many  kids  like  this  are  quite  talent ed,  so  they   excel  at  first  because  of  go od
                genes—but  then  they  hit  a  roadblock.  As  chess  struggl es  beco me  more  int ens e

                and  opponents  put  up  serious  resistance,  they   start  to  lose  interest  in  the   ga me.
                They  try  to  avoid  challenges,  but   eventual ly  the   real  world  finds   the m.  The ir
                confidence     is   fragile.   Losing   is   always   a   crisis   instead   of   an   oppo rtuni ty   for
                growth—if  they  were  a  winner  becau se  they   won,  thi s  new  losing  mus t  make

                them a loser.
                    The   long-term    effects   of   “opening   madnes s”   are   clear,   but    ther e   are   also
                serious   immediate     weaknesses    in   young   chess   pl ayers   br ought    up   in   thi s
                environment.  Just  as  there  are  inev itable  ups   and  do wns   in  a  career,  the re  are

                also  momentum  shifts  in  individual   games.  Most  of  my  early  rivals  were  gi fted
                children,  and they were prepared with  hundr eds of traps with  whi ch  the y coul d
   32   33   34   35   36   37   38   39   40   41   42