Page 165 - Malcolm Gladwell - Talking to Strangers
P. 165

Psychological Bulletin 115, no. 1 [1994]: 124.) So there they are, showing headshots to tribesmen of
                    white people making faces—and they are utterly reliant on their translator. They can’t just have each
                    tribesman free-associate about what he thinks is happening in each photo. How would they make
                    sense of that? They have to keep things simple. So Ekman and his group use what’s called “forced
                    choice.” They show each Fore person the pictures, one by one, and for every image they asked the
                    viewer to choose the right answer from a short list of emotions. Is what you are looking at anger,
                    sadness,  contempt,  disgust,  surprise,  happiness,  or  fear?  (The  Fore  didn’t  really  have  a  word  to
                    describe disgust or surprise, so the three researchers improvised: disgust was something that stinks;
                    surprise was something new.)
                    Now, is forced choice a good method? For example, suppose I want to find out whether you know
                    which city is the capital of Canada. (A surprising number of Americans, in my experience, have no
                    idea.) I could ask you straight out: What is the capital of Canada? That’s a free choice question. In
                    order to answer it correctly, you really have to know the capital of Canada. Now here’s the forced-
                    choice version of that question.
                       The capital of Canada is:
                           Washington, DC
                           Kuala Lumpur
                           Ottawa
                           Nairobi
                           Toronto
                    You can guess, can’t you? It’s not Washington, DC. Even someone with no knowledge whatsoever
                    of geography probably knows that’s the capital of the United States. It’s probably not Kuala Lumpur
                    or Nairobi, since those names don’t sound Canadian. So it’s down to Toronto or Ottawa. Even if you
                    have no idea what the capital of Canada is, you have a 50 percent chance of getting the answer right.
                    So is that what was happening with Ekman’s survey of the Fore?
                    Sergio Jarillo and Carlos Crivelli—the two researchers I write about in Chapter Six of this book—
                    began their research by attempting to replicate Ekman’s findings. Their idea was: let’s correct the
                    flaws in his exercise and see if it still holds up. Their first step was to pick a isolated tribe—the
                    Trobriand Islanders—whose language and culture at least one of them (Jarillo) knew well. That was
                    their first advantage over Ekman: they knew an awful lot more about whom they were talking to
                    than Ekman’s group had. They also decided not to use “forced choice.” They would use the far more
                    rigorous methodology of free choice. They laid out a set of headshots (with people looking happy,
                    sad, angry, scared, and disgusted) and asked, “Which of these is the sad face?” Then they asked the
                    next person, “Which of these is the angry face?” And so on. Finally, they tallied all the responses.
                    And what did they find? That when you redo Ekman’s foundational experiment—only this time,
                    carefully  and  rigorously—the  case  for  universalism  disappears.  Over  the  past  few  years  the
                    floodgates have opened, which is where much of  the research I  described in this chapter comes
                    from.
                    A few additional points:
                    Ekman’s original Science paper is, upon reflection, a little strange. He argued that what he found in
                    the  Fore  was  evidence  of  universalism.  But  if  you  examine  his  data,  it  doesn’t  look  like  he’s
                    describing universalism.
                    The Fore were really good at correctly identifying happy faces, but only about half of them correctly
                    identified  the  “fear”  face  as  being  an  expression  of  fear.  Forty-five  percent  of  them  thought  the
                    surprised  face  was  a  fearful  face.  Fifty-six  percent  of  them  read  sadness  as  anger.  This  is
                    universalism?
                    Crivelli made a very insightful remark when we were talking about the people (like Ekman) who so
                    favored  the  universalism  idea.  Many  of  them  belonged  to  the  generation  that  grew  up  in  the
                    aftermath of the Second World War. They were born into a world obsessed with human difference—
                    in which black people were thought to be genetically inferior and Jews were held to be damaged and
                    malignant—and they were powerfully drawn to a theory that maintained we are all the same.
                    It is important to note, however, that the work of anti-universalists is not a refutation of Ekman’s
                    contributions.  Everyone  in  the  field  of  human  emotion  is  in  some  crucial  sense  standing  on  his
   160   161   162   163   164   165   166   167   168   169   170