Page 320 - Zoo Animal Learning and Training
P. 320

292  12  Training Animals so They Can Return to the Wild

              What can we do to prevent northern quoll
  VetBooks.ir  extinctions?  One  promising  approach  is  to   inducing chemical  component).  The quolls
                                                     that consumed the toads became mildly ill,
            use conditioned taste aversion (CTA) to train
                                                     serious toad poisoning. Next, we offered
            quolls not to eat cane toads. CTA is a power-  and thankfully, none showed any signs of
            ful form of learning that occurs when animals   these trained quolls a small, live cane toad in
            ingest a novel, toxic food, experience illness,   an open topped jar, in front of a video cam-
            and subsequently associate the smell or taste   era,  to  see  whether  they  would  attack  the
            of that food with illness, and avoid ingesting   toads. Remarkably, the trained quolls sniffed
            the toxic food for long periods (Garcia et al.   the toad, but refused to attack it, whereas our
            1974). Unlike classical conditioning, whereby   control toad‐naive quolls quickly consumed
            animals require multiple trials to learn the   the live toad. The aversion to live cane toads
            appropriate response (Chapter 1), CTA usu-  lasted a week in captivity, suggesting that the
            ally involves one‐trial learning; that is, after a   toad‐trained quolls might have the skills
            single bout of illness, animals learn to avoid     necessary to survive in toad‐infested areas.
            the food that induced illness. After its initial   Did toad‐aversion training confer survival
            discovery, conservation biologists realised   benefits to quolls in toad‐infested habitats?
            that they could change the feeding behaviour   To answer  this question,  30 trained ‘toad‐
            of predators by adding nausea inducing   smart’ and 30 untrained ‘toad‐naive’ quolls
            chemicals to meat baits. Initial pen trials on   were  fitted  with  radio  collars  and  reintro-
            coyotes (Canis latrans) were encouraging;   duced to suitable rocky habitats in toad‐
            coyotes that consumed sheep baits paired   infested  areas  near  Darwin,  in  northern
            with  lithium chloride  became  ill, and  these   Australia (Figure 12.2). A large team of vol-
            coyotes subsequently refused to attack live   unteers was enlisted to help follow each quoll
            lambs (Gustavson et  al. 1974). Likewise,   during the first few hours after release, and
            crows that ate green‐coloured chicken eggs   locate them daily thereafter, so that we could
            injected with a nausea‐inducing chemical   determine their fate. The radio‐tracking
            subsequently refused to eat untreated green   work revealed that untrained quolls often
            chicken eggs, but continued to eat white eggs   encountered cane toads within hours of
            (Nicolaus et al. 1983) Despite these encourag-  release, and several of these toad‐naive quolls
            ing results, the use of CTA to alter predator   attacked large toads, and died from toad poi-
            behaviour was embroiled in controversy   soning. By contrast, the trained ‘toad‐smart’
            (Gustavson and Nicolaus 1987). Most of this   quolls encountered toads, sniffed them, and
            controversy surrounded the use of CTA baits
            to train wild coyotes to avoid attacking lambs;
            field trials produced equivocal results
            (Bourne and Dorrance 1982) and the use of
            CTA was abandoned in favour of alternative
            (and often, lethal) methods to control coyote
            numbers (Gustavson and Nicolaus 1987;
            Conover and Kessler 1994). Thus, until
            recently CTA has largely been overlooked as a
            tool for training animals.
              To see if we could train quolls not to eat
            cane toads, in 2009 my colleagues and I fed
            toad‐naive, captive reared quolls a small,
            non‐lethal sized (<2 g) dead toad infused   Figure 12.2  Photograph of a trained northern quoll
            with the odourless, tasteless, nausea‐induc-  fitted with a radio‐collar. Both untrained and trained
                                                     northern quolls were released in the wet season at
            ing chemical thiabendazole (a chemical used   sites around Darwin, where toads were abundant.
            to deworm livestock which has a nausea   Source: Jonathan Webb.
   315   316   317   318   319   320   321   322   323   324   325