Page 321 - Zoo Animal Learning and Training
P. 321

12.3   Training Northern Quolls to Avoid Eating Cane Toads  293

  VetBooks.ir  rejected them as prey, and consequently, the   captive reared toad‐trained female quolls
                                                      successfully raised litters of young, and
             trained quolls had higher survival than the
             control group during the 10 day monitoring
                                                      revealed that the offspring of one of these
             period (O’Donnell et al. 2010). Encouragingly,   importantly, DNA analyses of parentage
             several trained quolls that we reintroduced   females also survived and reproduced
             to Mary River Park, a tourist accommodation   (Cremona et al. 2017b). Thus, some juvenile
             site surrounded by monsoonal vine thicket   quolls learnt to avoid eating cane toads, and
             and savannah woodland, survived long term,   social  learning may be  important in this
             and one female survived to breeding age and   respect. Quolls have a high degree of mater-
             raised a family of quolls in the caravan park   nal care, and the young often travel on their
             owner’s machinery shed.                  mother’s backs at night. By radio‐tracking
               These encouraging results suggested that it   female quolls and their offspring, and plac-
             might be possible to reintroduce ‘toad‐smart’   ing remotely triggered infrared cameras near
             quolls to toad‐infested sites where northern   the den sites of mothers when their offspring
             quoll populations have crashed or gone   were denning independently, we discovered
             locally extinct. To test this idea, 50 captive‐  close associations between juveniles and
             born quolls (28 males, 22 females) from the   mothers. Notably, young quolls that were
             Territory Wildlife  Park were  trained not  to   denning independently of their mother were
             eat toads by feeding them a small (<2 g) dead   photographed with their mother foraging at
             toad coated with thiabendazole at a dose rate   night (Figure 12.3), suggesting that juveniles
                        −1
             of  300 mg kg   predator  mass.  The  quolls   might have opportunities to learn what to
             were released to suitable habitats (rock out-  eat, and what not to eat, by observing their
             crops) near East Alligator Ranger Station in   ‘toad‐smart’ mothers hunting prey and sniff-
             Kakadu National Park. Previous studies   ing and rejecting toads as prey. Whether
             showed that quolls were abundant at East   such information is transmitted to offspring
             Alligator prior to the arrival of cane toads,   remains unknown, but seems likely (Galef
             but the population crashed after toads   and Laland 2005). Indeed, the extended
             invaded  and  was  on  a  path  to  extinction   duration of maternal care in quolls provides
             (Oakwood and Foster 2008; Woinarski et al.   an opportunity for social learning in this
             2010). The longer‐term survival of trained   species. Alternatively, juvenile quolls might
             ‘toad‐smart’ quolls was monitored by trap-  naturally learn to avoid eating toads by
             ping three times each year (usually, in March,   ingesting small non‐lethal sized toads that
             May, and November) over four years. Tissue   induce nausea and long‐lasting aversions in
             samples were taken from all recaptured   other small carnivorous marsupials (Webb
             quolls, and DNA parentage analyses were   et al. 2008, 2011).
             run to determine the identity of the mothers   Although for the second generation of
             and fathers of juveniles in the population.  quolls bred in the wild, the reintroduction
               Monitoring of quolls revealed that toad   was not successful. Even though cane toads
             aversion training conferred long‐term bene-  were no longer a major source of mortality
             fits for some quolls. Most males disappeared   for  toad‐trained  quolls,  predation  by  free‐
             from the study sites shortly after release, but   ranging domesticated dogs and dingoes were
             four males that were recaptured survived for   major sources of mortality for quolls on the
             an average five months (range 1–10 months).   study site. Indeed, predation by canids is
             Males typically only live for a year, so these   probably preventing the reintroduced popu-
             males survived long enough to breed with   lation of quolls from recovering (Cremona
             females. Females survived longer than males,   et al. 2017). Unfortunately, the loss of tradi-
             and seven females that were recaptured on   tional burning practices in Kakadu has led to
             the study site survived for an average of nine   an increase in hot late dry season fires
             months (range 2–22 months). Three of these   (Russell‐Smith 2016); these fires incinerate
   316   317   318   319   320   321   322   323   324   325   326