Page 432 - Deep Learning
P. 432

Notes to Pages 79–82                  415

                the strategies of one domain seem to be unrelated to those of other domains,
                has discouraged a search for a general theory of problem-solving strategies (but
                see Lenat, 1983, for a promising start). Perhaps due to the difficulty of finding
                anything general to say, research that primarily aims to identify and analyze
                problem-solving strategies has waned as a basic research enterprise, but con-
                tinues to be of interest in applied contexts; see, e.g., Schraagen, Chipman and
                Shalin (2000). For a strategy-oriented application of search concepts to under-
                standing creativity in Artificial Intelligence, see Buchanan (2001): “Search at the
                meta-level gives us a means for identifying the choices that are most effective for
                performing a specific task” (p. 23). This top-down view of (artificial) creativity
                is almost the opposite of the psychological theory proposed in Chapter 4 of this
                volume.
              52.  Hayes and Simon (1974) and Simon and Hayes (1976) did empirical work on the
                comprehension of problem instructions, but their conclusions fell far short of a
                theory of the origin of problem spaces. Buchanan’s (2001) concept of meta-level
                search is one vision of such a theory.
              53.  For  a  history  of  Gestalt  psychology,  see  Ash  (1995);  for  many  of  the  original
                papers, see Ellis (1967); and for a reconstruction of their theory of problem solv-
                ing, see Ohlsson (1984a).
              54.  The Gestalt laws are summarized in Goldstein (2008, pp. 72–80). The original
                texts are Koffka (1922) and Wertheimer (1923).
              55.  The Necker Cube was first published by the Swiss crystallographer Louis Albert
                Necker in 1832 in the London and Edinburgh Philosophical Magazine and Journal
                of Science (Necker, 1832) and again the following year in Annalen der Physik.
                While studying line drawings of crystals Necker discovered the tendency of such
                drawings to flip between two alternative three-dimensional (3D) percepts. How
                the Necker Cube passed from these science journals into Gestalt psychology I
                do not know, but it is mentioned in an article on the physiology of vision by
                Wheatstone (1838, pp. 381–382). The closely related figure-ground distinction and
                the use of reversible figures to illustrate it are due to the Danish psychologist
                Edgar Rubin, whose Danish-language book Synopslevede Figurer (Copenhagen,
                Denmark: Gyldendalske, 1915) appears not to have been translated into English.
                Reversible figures were discussed by the Gestalt psychologists and passed into
                common knowledge, but knowledge of the original sources was lost along the
                way. The reversions of the Necker Cube and its relatives continue to serve as
                symbols or metaphors for representational change in general. For example, Kuhn
                (1970) drew an analogy between reversible figures and paradigm shifts in sci-
                ence (pp. 111–114). The features of the Necker Cube that continue to fascinate
                are that a 3D perception of the line drawing is clearly a cognitive construction,
                and that a shift between the two possible 3D constructions is just as clearly non-
                  monotonic: It is impossible to see the two 3D interpretations of the drawing at
                the same time. The Necker Cube continues to interest researchers in visual per-
                ception as well, now absorbed into a wider research program on “multistability”
                and “binocular rivalry” (Pearson & Clifford, 2004).
              56.  Wertheimer (1959/1968, pp. 170–171).
              57.  Köhler (1972, p. 134).
   427   428   429   430   431   432   433   434   435   436   437