Page 434 - Deep Learning
P. 434

Notes to Pages 90–95                 417

                memory  capacity  is  related  to  analytical  problem  solving  but  not  to  insight
                problem solving.
              11.  The insight sequence is superficially similar to the four-stage theory of illumina-
                tion proposed by Graham Wallas (1926). The main difference is that the insight
                sequence describes problem solving without a pause, while Wallas’s stages include
                an incubation period in which the problem solver rests. Incubation is discussed
                further at the end of this chapter.
              12.  See, e.g., Durkin (1937), Fleck and Weisberg (2004), Maier (1931), Metcalfe (1986),
                Metcalfe and Wiebe (1987) and Ohlsson (1990c).
              13.  Brain-imaging techniques enable researchers to capture moments of insight; see
                Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003); Bowden, Jung-Beeman, Fleck and Kounios
                (2005);  Jung-Beeman  et  al.  (2004),  Kounios  et  al.  (2006);  Luo  and  Knoblich
                (2007);  Luo,  Niki  and  Knoblich  (2006);  and  Sandkühler  and  Bhattacharyya
                (2008).
              14.  The classical self-report is Poincaré’s autobiographical account of a mathemati-
                cal discovery, already discussed in Chapter 3 (Poincaré, 1908/1952), but similar
                events have been reported by many others; see Ghiselin (1952) for a collection of
                testimonies from artists and writers and Hadamard (1949/1954) for an analysis
                of self-reports from mathematicians. Examples are mentioned in Chapter 5; see
                Table 5.1.
              15.  Using a strict criterion of cessation of all activity to identify impasses, Fleck and
                Weisberg (2004) found only a single example of the full insight sequence (impasse,
                restructuring and successful solution) among 34 subjects who attempted to solve
                Duncker’s Candle Problem. However, 13 additional subjects experienced both
                impasse and restructuring but did not solve the problem within the time limit set
                by the experimenters. Depending on which number is used, these data estimate
                the prevalence of the insight sequence as either 3% or 41%; at either frequency, the
                phenomenon is of interest.
              16.  The idea that perception is an interpretive process and that consequently there
                are no theory-free observations – instead, observations are theory-laden – was
                discussed by Hanson (1965), who used the term “theory loaded” as well as “the-
                ory laden”; only the latter seems to have survived in philosophical discourse. The
                concept that observations are theory laden is widely accepted in the philosophy
                of science. Brewer and Lampert (2001) analyzed this concept from a cognitive
                point  of  view.  A  classical  psychological  work  that  emphasizes  the  complexity
                of perceptual processing is Neisser (1967). The point is equally visible from an
                Artificial Intelligence perspective (Marr, 1982).
              17.  Textbooks  on  sensation  and  perception  make  this  abundantly  clear  (Levine,
                2000; Wolfe et al., 2006). See also Biederman (1987) and Marr (1982) for con-
                structive theories of particular aspects of vision.
              18.  “The  whole  visual  system  can  be  considered  a  long  series  of  filters,  with
                each  stage  in  the  system  responsible  for  extracting  a  particular  aspect  of  the
                visual world and passing this aspect on to the next stage” (Wolfe et al., 2006,
                p. 40).
              19.  Wolfe et al. (2006, pp. 93–94).
              20.  Kintsch (1988).
   429   430   431   432   433   434   435   436   437   438   439